검색 전체 메뉴
PDF
맨 위로
OA 학술지
Symmetry Detection of 3D Objects*
  • 비영리 CC BY-NC
ABSTRACT
Symmetry Detection of 3D Objects*
KEYWORD
Symmetry perception , three-dimensional stimuli , presentation duration , stimuli complexity , 3D stimulus creation , 3D Studio Max
  • 1. Introduction

    Forming coherent mental representations of visual objects is an integral part of the visual system (Peissig & Tarr, 2007). Such representations involve both perceptual and cognitive processes and provide essential information for understanding our visual environments (Palmeri & Gau thier, 2004). Research on visual memory (Cooper & Schacter, 1992; Loftus, Miller, & Burns, 1978; Loftus & Palmer, 1974; Schacter & Cooper, 1993; Schacter et al., 1995; Squire, 1998), visual object classification (Mack, Gauthier, Sadr, & Palmeri, 2008), symmetry perception (Driver, Baylis, & Rafal, 1992; Yamauchi et al., 2006), and face recognition (Bukach, Gauthier, & Tarr, 2006; Gauthier & Bukach, 2007; McKone, Kanwisher, & Duchaine, 2006) are all linked to the central issues of object representation (Biederman & Bar, 1999; Biederman & Cooper, 1991, 1992; Marr, 1981; Marr & Nishihara, 1978; Peissig & Tarr, 2007; Poggio & Edelman, 1990; Tarr, 2003; Vuong & Tarr, 2004).

    One technical problem blocking the progress of this line of research is the difficulty of developing realistic three-dimensional stimuli. A majority of visual perception and cognition studies have employed two-dimensional line drawings or simple pictures of three-dimensional objects as stimuli (Biederman & Bar, 1999; Biederman & Gerhardstein, 1993; Gauthier & Tarr, 1997b; Hayward & Tarr, 1997; Subramaniam, Biederman, & Madigan, 2000; but see Gauthier & Tarr, 1997a for some exceptions). The problem is that neurons in the visual cortex are highly interconnected, and, often times, these neurons respond differently in natural viewing conditions (Carandini et al., 2005, p. 10588; Felsen & Dan, 2005; Yuille & Kersten, 2006; Smyth, Willmore, Baker, Thompson, & Tolhurst, 2003; Touryan, Felsen, & Dan, 2005). In this regard, investigating behavioral responses to simple stimuli such as edges, blobs, bars, or line drawings may not give us the accurate picture of the visual system in question (Barlow, 2001; Felsen & Dan, 2005; Simoncelli & Olshausen, 2001).

    This problem is particularly serious in symmetry perception research. Symmetries of objects are pivotal in both structural description models and view-based models of object representation. Research has shown that visual symmetries provide strong cues for image segmentation, such as distinguishing a figure from its background (Driver et al., 1992). The consensus is that symmetry perception occurs before and/or separately from the representation of local components and their configurations (Driver et al., 1992; but see Olibers & Helm, 1998). Evidence suggests that the complexity of local image features hardly interferes with the speed and accuracy of symmetry perception of two-dimensional figures (i.e., reflectional symmetry; see Baylis & Driver, 1994; Wagemans, 1995, 1997).

    However, these claims were limited by results based on simplified twodimensional stimuli (e.g., Baylis & Driver, 1994). Recently, although some progress was made in the field of symmetry perception with three-dimensional objects (e.g., Niimi & Yokosawa, 2009), most findings of symmetry perception have been performed with two-dimensional objects. To remedy this problem, we developed an archive of three-dimensional stimuli using 3D Studio Max (see Appendices A & B for stimulus archive and tutorial) and investigated the nature of symmetry perception with realistic threedimensional stimuli (see Figure 1 for sample stimuli).

       1.1 Symmetry and object representation

    In Biederman’s (Biederman, 1987; Hummel & Biederman, 1992) recognition-by-components theory, the visual system extracts two-dimensional edge properties such as curvature, co-linearity, symmetry, parallelism, and/or co-termination from image data, and then constructs three-dimensional geometric primitives from these edge features (see also Layer 2 of “JIM” on p. 486, Hummel & Biederman, 1992). Similarly, in the Marr and Nishihara model (1978), axes of symmetry are computed in the service of generating a structural description of object shape. These models suggest that some form of symmetry detection occurs prior to the representation of local components and their structure. Consistent with these models, evidence suggests that symmetry perception can be achieved without the complete component-based structural descriptions of objects (Baylis & Driver, 1994; Yamauchi et al., 2006).

    However, it is unknown whether the same process is involved in symmetry perception of realistic three-dimensional stimuli. Symmetry may emerge spontaneously without attention for simple two-dimensional stimuli (Driver et al., 1992 but see Olibers & Helm, 1998); yet, a more elaborate mechanism may be required for realistic three-dimensional objects. The construction of object structure by three-dimensional local components involves the binding of image features to objects, and this binding process is incomplete unless attention is given to the relevant locations (Logan, 1990; Treisman & Kanwisher, 1998; Stankiewicz, Hummel, & E. E. Cooper, 1998). This means that symmetry perception for three-dimensional objects can be an incremental process, where symmetry is extracted gradually as the global structure of objects is recovered. We tested this idea in two experiments.

    In Experiment 1, we examined the accuracy and response times of participants judging the symmetry or asymmetry of individual stimuli, where the complexity of stimuli and the duration of stimulus presentation were manipulated. We varied the number of local components of individual stimuli and examined how the complexity of stimuli, as measured by the number of distinct components that each object had, interacted with the accuracy and speed of symmetry perception. As observed in two-dimensional stimuli, if symmetry perception occurs spontaneously in realistic three-dimensional stimuli, the accuracy and response times of symmetry perception should be relatively independent of the duration of stimulus presentation and the complexity of stimuli. Contrary to this view, the results of Experiment 1 show that symmetry perception for three-dimensional stimuli is highly reliant on the duration of stimulus presentation and the complexity of stimuli. In Experiment 2, we implemented an incidental recognition memory experiment and examined whether stimulus complexity, as defined in Experiment 1, would also influence recognition memory performance. The results of Experiment 2 show that the stimulus complexity influences recognition memory performance when the study task involves incidental encoding of the global structures of objects.

    2. Experiment 1

    Experiment 1 examined the extent to which the presentation duration and the complexity of stimuli influence symmetry detection of threedimensional objects. The hypothesis that symmetry perception for threedimensional objects requires a gradual recovery of 3D structures predicts that presentation duration and stimulus complexity influence symmetry detection considerably. To test this prediction, participants judged the symmetry/asymmetry of stimuli in 5 different time durations (30, 40, 50, or 60 millisecond), as well as without time limitations. The complexity of stimuli was also manipulated by varying the number of distinct local components of which individual stimuli were composed.

       2.1 Methods

    2.1.1 Participants

    All participants (N = 213) were Texas A & M undergraduate students who participated in this study for course credit. The participants were assigned randomly to one of five stimulus presentation duration conditions; (30ms, n = 42; 40ms, n = 46; 50ms, n = 46; 60ms, n = 43; unlimited exposure, n = 36). All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision.

    2.1.2 Stimuli and procedure

    Sixty symmetric objects and 60 asymmetric objects (Figure 1) were created by the 3ds Max program, release 6 [by Autodesk (Kinetix), San Rafael, California, USA]. To compose stimuli, we applied a number of modification techniques (described in detail in Appendix B), such as union, intersection, subtraction as well as lofting to 18 different kinds of primitive components (Figure 2). On average, our stimuli consisted of 4.6 components (4.9 and 4.3 components for symmetric and asymmetric objects respectively). The symmetric objects used in this study had a maximum of 11 components, and the asymmetric objects had a maximum of 10 components. The minimum numbers of components used in the symmetric and asymmetric objects were 2 and 1, respectively.1

    The stimuli extended approximately 4.0 degrees of visual angle.2 The procedural details of stimulus generation are described in Appendices A and B; the tutorial is based on 3ds Max, release 10.

    The task for participants was to indicate whether stimuli shown on the computer screen were symmetric or asymmetric by pressing designated keys. To start each trial, participants pressed the space bar, which generated a fixation cross, visible for 500 milliseconds. After the fixation cross disappeared, a stimulus was presented and remained at the center of the computer monitor until participants responded. To indicate the symmetry/asymmetry of the stimulus, participants pressed the left or right arrow key. Immediately after participants pressed one of the arrow keys, the monitor displayed a sign indicating a directive to press the space bar, which led to the presentation of the fixation cross and the next stimulus. The stimuli were presented by a Dell OptiPlex GX240 with Pentium IV processors (2.4 GHz) on a Gateway vx730 monitor with a resolution of 1280 x 1024 pixels.

    2.1.3 Design

    The experiment had a 5 (presentation duration; 30ms, 40ms, 50ms, 60ms, unlimited, between-subjects) x 2 (stimulus complexity; low, high) factorial design. The dependent measure was detection accuracy (d’) and response times.3 For the detection accuracy measure, we defined “hit” as responding “symmetry” given symmetric objects, and “false alarm” as responding “symmetry” given asymmetric objects (see Barlow & Reeves, 1979; Wenderoth, 1997 for a similar procedure). Calculated this way, this measure helps control possible response bias (Ratcliff & McKoon, 1995; Roediger & McDermott, 1994). Because detection accuracy differed drastically in each presentation duration condition, the response times were measured for all trials rather than just the trials with correct responses.

    To assess the impact of stimulus complexity, we measured the number of distinct local components that each stimuli had. This measure yielded two stimulus complexity categories: low and high complexity. For asymmetric objects, low complexity objects had 1 or 2 distinct kinds of components and high complexity objects had 3 to 5 distinct components (low complexity objects, n = 34; high complexity objects, n = 26 objects). For symmetric objects, low complexity objects had 1 or 2 distinct kinds of components and high complexity objects had 3 or 4 distinct kinds of components (low complexity objects, n = 33; high complexity objects, n = 27 objects).

       2.2 Results

    Detection accuracy (d’). The impact of presentation durations on symmetry detection is first reported and followed by the relationship between the stimulus complexity and detection accuracy. As shown in Figure 3, a 5 (presentation duration; 30ms, 40ms, 50ms, 60ms, unlimited) x 2 (complexity; high vs. low) ANOVA revealed that there was a significant main effect of presentation duration; F(4, 208) = 87.34, MSE = 0.04, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.627. A contrast analysis applied to the 4 durations [the 30, 40, 50, 60 milliseconds presentation durations with contrast coefficients of (-3, -1, 1, 3)] reveals that there is a significant linear trend between the presentation duration factor and detection accuracy (d’), t(173) = 7.38, p < 0.001. The presentation duration factor did not influence the response time measure for both symmetric and asymmetric objects; F’s < 1.0.

    There was also a reliable main effect of stimulus complexity; F(1, 208) = 23.80, MSE = 0.186, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.103, indicating that participants were more accurate for low-complexity stimuli than high complexity stimuli. The interaction between complexity and presentation duration was also significant; F(4, 208) = 2.64, p < 0.035, η2 = 0.048. Planned comparisons suggest that symmetries of low complexity stimuli were detected more accurately than those of high complexity stimuli in the 60ms presentation duration and in the unlimited presentation duration; 60ms, t(42) = 3.76, p < 0.01; unlimited, t(35) = 3.68, p < 0.01 (Figure 3a).

    Response time. The complexity factor also influenced the response time measure for symmetric, but not asymmetric, objects. The average response time for high complexity stimuli was longer than low complexity stimuli in symmetric objects; F(1, 208) = 14.96, MSE = 19239.7, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.067, and the interaction between presentation duration and complexity was also significant; F(4, 208) = 3.65, MSE = 19239.7, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.066. This interaction effect stemmed exclusively from the unlimited time exposure condition, t(35) = 6.84, p < 0.01. The complexity factor did not influence the response times of asymmetric objects; F(1, 208) = 1.48, MSE = 58798.0, p = 0. 225, η2 = 0.007. Complexity x duration; F(4, 208) = 1.87, MSE = 58798.0, p = 0.12, η2 = 0.035.

    Taken together, these results show that symmetry detection for threedimensional objects depends on the duration of stimulus presentation and the complexity of stimuli, suggesting that detection of symmetries of 3D objects is unlikely to be spontaneous and preattentative.

    1The “primitive components” used in our stimuli are defined and produced strictly in the 3ds Max program, and “primitives” are different from geometric primitives/parts as defined and characterized by Biederman (1987) or by Hoffman and Richards (1984).  2All stimuli and data for individual objects can be downloaded from http://people.tamu.edu/~takashi-yamauchi/stimuli/ and http://people.tamu.edu/~takashiyamauchi/stimuli/data/  3Hit and false alarm scores equal to 1 or 0 were converted to 0.99 and 0.01, respectively, in order to calculate Z-scores.

    3. Experiment 2

    In Experiment 2, we investigated the relationship between symmetry perception and “stimulus complexity” indirectly in a recognition memory test. Previous studies investigating implicit memory of three-dimensional objects suggest that global structures of objects such as orientation axes of symmetry can be formed after an encoding task in which participants judged the left-right orientation of objects (Liu & Cooper, 2001; Yamauchi et al., 2006). This encoding task is also known to produce priming in symmetry/asymmetry judgments. Thus, if the stimulus complexity that is defined in Experiment 1 is psychologically real, it should affect the performance for the incidental recognition memory task. We tested this idea.

    The experiment consisted of two phases: a study phase and a test phase. During the study phase, participants studied left-right orientations of 30 symmetric and 30 asymmetric objects. After the study phase they were given a recognition task in the test phase. This experiment was conducted incidentally in that participants were not aware of the intent of the experiment during the study phase (an incidental test). In an additional follow-up study, we also investigated the relationship between stimulus complexity and intentional recognition memory performance. The intent of this follow-up study is explained in the Result section.

       3.1 Methods

    3.1.1 Participants

    A total of 60 undergraduate students (female = 40; male = 20) from Texas A & M University participated in Experiment 2 for course credit. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity.

    3.1.2 Stimuli and procedure

    The materials used in this experiment were identical to those described in Experiment 1. Of the total 60 symmetric objects and 60 asymmetric objects, 30 asymmetric objects and 30 symmetric objects were shown in the study phase, and all 120 (60 symmetric and 60 asymmetric objects) were shown in the test phase. We developed two versions of stimuli for counterbalancing.

    In the study phase, 30 symmetric objects and 30 asymmetric objects were randomly presented one at a time at the centre of the screen for five seconds each. Participants examined each object as a whole for the entire 5 seconds and judged whether the object faced primarily to the right or to the left by pressing one of two specified keys (see Schacter et al., 1990, for this task). No mention was made at this point about the subsequent recognition task. Shortly after the study phase, participants proceeded to a recognition task, in which participants made ‘yes/no’ recognition responses for studied and non-studied objects.

    3.1.3 Design

    The main dependent measure was d’ calculated from hit and false alarm scores obtained in the recognition memory task. “Hit” is defined as the probability of responding “old” given that the stimulus appeared during the study phase (p(response = “old” | old)) and “false alarm” is defined as the probability of responding “old” given that the stimulus did not appear during the study phase (p(response = “old” | new). To analyze the relationship between stimulus complexity and recognition memory, an item-based linear regression analysis was applied to the data with the stimulus complexity measures (i.e., the number of distinct local components) as a predictor variable.

       3.2 Results

    Table 1 summarizes the results from the recognition memory test. The average hit minus false alarm scores of asymmetric objects and symmetric objects were 0.24 and 0.26, respectively. In both cases, these scores were significantly higher than a chance level, indicating that participants were able to distinguish individual objects in their recognition memory; asymmetric objects, t(59) = 12.7, p < 0.001; symmetric objects, t(59) = 13.0, p < 0.001.

    As predicted, a linear regression analysis applied to individual objects revealed that there is a significant linear relationship between stimulus complexity and recognition memory performance when the study task was given incidentally; F(1, 118) = 11.87, MSE = 0.02, p < 0.01; β = -0.30, p < 0.01.

    [Table 1.] A summary of the recognition memory tests in Experiment 2

    label

    A summary of the recognition memory tests in Experiment 2

    4. Follow-up Test

    In a follow-up study (N = 30), we also tested the effect of stimulus complexity in a recognition memory experiment in which participants were instructed to study individual objects carefully for the subsequent memory test (i.e., an intentional recognition memory task). The purpose of this follow-up study was to check the possibility that the effect of stimulus complexity stems from the left-right orientation task in which axes of symmetry were encoded, rather than the member encoding in general.

       4.1 Methods

    4.1.1 Participants

    A total of 30 undergraduate students from Texas A & M University participated in Experiment 3 for course credit. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity.

    4.1.2 Stimuli and Procedure

    The materials used in this experiment were identical to those described in Experiment 2. Participants in this follow-up study did not carry out the leftright orientation task for individual stimuli during the study phase; instead, participants actively studied stimuli for a subsequent memory task in any way they wanted. Studies showed that the intentional memory task leads people to form semantic associations of stimuli (Cooper, Schacter, Ballesteros, & Moore, 1992; Schacter et al., 1990; Schacter & Cooper, 1993). In this regard, it was expected that the stimulus complexity as measured by the number of distinct components would not relate to participants’ recognition performance in this case.

    4.1.3 Design

    The main dependent measure was the same as Experiment 2.

       4.2 Results

    As expected, there was no statistically significant link between the stimulus complexity variable and the recognition memory performance in the intentional memory task; F < 1.0.

    5. General Discussion

    The results from these two experiments indicate that there is a strong possibility that symmetry perception for three-dimensional objects is an incremental process, where symmetry is extracted gradually as the global structure of objects is recovered. In Experiment 1, we reasoned that if symmetry perception emerges gradually as global three-dimensional information is recovered from the image data then symmetry detection should be influenced by the complexity and presentation durations of stimuli. The results of Experiment 1 were consistent with this view. Experiment 2 employed an incidental memory task. In this setting, participants judged the left-right orientation of stimulus objects during the study phase and later tested their recognition memory of these objects in the test phase. As predicted, the results of Experiment 2 revealed that recognition memory performance was highly correlated with stimulus complexity when the study task involved incidental left-right orientation judgments, but not when the study task involved intentional encoding. Taken together, these results are consistent with the view that symmetry perception in three-dimensional objects emerges in an incremental process, where symmetry is extracted gradually as the global structure of objects is recovered.

       5.1 Implications: Symmetry Perception and Object Representation

    Clearly, at the very early stage of object representation, some form of part decomposition and the assignment of edges to a figure should occur. At this stage, some form of coarse descriptions of object parts occurs (e.g., Hoffman & Ridchards, 1984), and this coarse representation of object parts probably suffices for initial symmetry detection relevant to two-dimensional images. After this stage, symmetry perception for three-dimensional stimuli arises gradually as figure-ground segregation is achieved (see also Sekuler & Palmer, 1992; van Lier, Leeuwenberg, & Helm, 1997). We suggest that symmetry detection is likely to go through multiple-stages (Wagemans, 1997). Given the current finding that symmetry perception for three-dimensional stimuli depends on stimulus complexity and presentation durations, the later stages of symmetry perception may employ a constellation of features as cues determining the axis of symmetry, which can be identified, for example, by processing local energy functions (the intensity of pixels) and then local maxima of a convoluted image. This process proceeds iteratively as more fine-tuned features are recovered from the original images (e.g., Scognamillo, Rhodes, Morrone, & Burr, 2003).

    In Biederman and Hummel’s model of object representation and recognition (Biederman, 1987; Hummel & Biederman, 1992; see also Marr, 1981; Marr & Nishihara, 1978), a limited number of geometric primitives and their configurations render the representation of a variety of basiclevel objects. The model suggests that constructing three-dimensional local components from two-dimensional non-accidental image data requires the detection and integration of different image features (Biederman, 1987). In order to combine these properties, some attentional mechanisms may be deployed. We suggest that symmetry perception for three-dimensional objects appear as these local three dimensional features are recovered gradually. Future research should investigate exactly how the recovery of threedimensional features eventually gives rise to the extraction of global axes of symmetry.

참고문헌
  • 1. Barlow H. 2001 Redundancy reduction revisited. [Network: Computation in Neural Systems] Vol.12 P.241-253 google
  • 2. Barlow H., Reeves B. C. 1979 The versatility and absolute efficiency of detecting mirror symmetry in random dot displays. [Vision Research] Vol.19 P.783-793 google cross ref
  • 3. Baylis G. C., Driver J. 1994 Parallel computation of symmetry but not repetition within single visual shapes. [Visual Cognition] Vol.1 P.377-400 google cross ref
  • 4. Biederman I. 1987 Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding. [Psychological Review] Vol.94 P.115-147 google cross ref
  • 5. Biederman I., Bar M. 1999 One-shot viewpoint invariance in matching novel objects. [Vision Research] Vol.39 P.2885-2889 google cross ref
  • 6. Biederman I., Cooper E. E. 1991 Priming contour-deleted images: evidence for intermediate representations in visual object recognition. [Cognitive Psychology] Vol.23 P.393-419 google cross ref
  • 7. Biederman I., Cooper E. E. 1992 Size invariance in visual object priming. [Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance] Vol.18 P.121-133 google cross ref
  • 8. Biederman I., Gerhardstein P. C. 1993 Recognizing depth-rotated objects: Evidence and conditions for 3D viewpoint invariance. [Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance] Vol.19 P.1162-1182 google cross ref
  • 9. Bukach C. M., Gauthier I., Tarr M. J. 2006 Beyond faces and modularity: The power of an expertise framework. [Trends in Cognitive Science] Vol.10 P.159-66 google cross ref
  • 10. Carandini M., Demb J. B., Mante V., Olshausen R. A., Tolhurst D. J., Dan Y., Gallant J. L., Rust N. 2005 “Do we know what the early visual system does?” [Journal of Neurosciece] Vol.25 P.10577-10597 google cross ref
  • 11. Cooper L. A., Schacter D. L. 1992 Dissociations between structural and episodic representations of visual objects. [Current Directions in Psychological Science] Vol.1 P.141-146 google cross ref
  • 12. Cooper L. A., Schacter D. L., Ballesteros S., Moore C. 1992 Priming and recognition of transformed three-dimensional objects: Effects of size and reflection. [Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition] Vol.18 P.43-57 google cross ref
  • 13. Corballis M., Roldan C. 1974 perception of symmetrical and repeated patterns. [Perception and Psychophysics] Vol.16 P.136-142 google cross ref
  • 14. Driver J., Baylis G. C., Rafal R. D. 1992 Preserved figure-ground segregation and symmetry perception in visual neglect. [Nature] Vol.360 P.73-75 google cross ref
  • 15. Felsen G., Dan Y. 2005 A natural approach to studying vision. [Naure. Neuroscience] Vol.8 P.1643-1646 google
  • 16. Gauthier I., Bukach C. 2007 Should we reject the expertise hypothesis? [Cognition] Vol.103 P.322-330 google cross ref
  • 17. Gauthier I., Tarr M. J. 1997a Becoming a “Greeble expert”: Exploring the face recognition mechanism. [Vision Research] Vol.37 P.1673-1682 google
  • 18. Gauthier I., Tarr M. J. 1997b Orientation priming of novel shapes in the context of viewpoint-dependent recognition. [Perception] Vol.26 P.51-73 google cross ref
  • 19. Hayward W. G., Tarr M. J. 1997 Testing conditions for viewpoint invariance in object recognition. [Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance] Vol.23 P.1511-1521 google cross ref
  • 20. Hoffman D. D., Richards W. A. 1984 Parts of recognition. [Cognition] Vol.18 P.65-96 google cross ref
  • 21. Hummel J. E., Biederman I. 1992 Dynamic binding in a neural network for shape recognition. [Psychological Review] Vol.99 P.480-517 google cross ref
  • 22. Liu T., Cooper L. A. 2001 The influence of task requirements on priming in object decision and matching. [Memory & Cognition] Vol.29 P.874-882 google cross ref
  • 23. Loftus E. F., Miller D. G., Burns H. J. 1978 Semantic integration of verbal information into a visual memory. [Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory] Vol.4 P.19-31 google cross ref
  • 24. Loftus E. F., Palmer J. C. 1974 Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory. [Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior] Vol.13 P.585-589 google cross ref
  • 25. Logan G. D. 1994 Spatial attention and the apprehension of spatial relations. [Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance] Vol.20 P.1015-1036 google cross ref
  • 26. Mack M., Gauthier I., Sadr J., Palmeri T. J. 2008 Object detection and basiclevel categorization: Sometimes you know it is there before you know what it is. [Psychonomic Bulletin & Review] Vol.15 P.28-35 google cross ref
  • 27. Marr D. 1981 Vision. google
  • 28. Marr D., Nishihara H. K. 1978 Representation and recognition of the spatial organization of three-dimensional shapes. [Proceedings of the Royal Society of London] Vol.200 P.269-294 google cross ref
  • 29. Marslen-Wilson W. D., Bozic M., Randall B. 2008 Early decomposition in visual word recognition: Dissociating morphology, form, and meaning. [Language and Cognitive Processes] Vol.23 P.394-421 google cross ref
  • 30. McKone E., Kanwisher N., Duchaine B. C. 2006 Can generic expertise explain special processing for faces? [Trends in Cognitive Science] Vol.11 P.8-15 google cross ref
  • 31. Niimi R., Yokosawa K. 2009 Viewpoint dependence in the recognition of nonelongated familiar objects: testing the effects of symmetry, front-back axis, and familiarity. [Perception] Vol.38 P.533-551 google cross ref
  • 32. Olibers C. N. L., van der Helm P. A. 1998 Symmetry and selective attention: A dissociation between effortless perception and serial search. [Perception and Psychophysics] Vol.60 P.1101-1116 google cross ref
  • 33. Palmer S. E. 1991 Goodness, gestalt, groups, and Garner: Local symmetry subgroups as a theory of figural goodness. In G. R. Lockhead & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), The perception of structure: Essays in honor of Wendell R. Garner P.23-39 google
  • 34. Palmer S. E., Hemenway K. 1978 Orientation and symmetry: Effects of multiple, rotational, and new symmetries. [Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance] Vol.4 P.691-702 google cross ref
  • 35. Palmeri T. J., Gauthier I. 2004 Visual object understanding. [Nature Reviews Neuroscience] Vol.5 P.291-303 google cross ref
  • 36. Peissig J. J., Tarr M. J. 2007 Visual object recognition: Do we know more now than we did 20 years ago? In S. T. Fiske (Eds.) [Annual Review of Psychology] Vol.58 P.75-96 google cross ref
  • 37. Poggio T., Edelman S. 1990 A network that learns to recognize threedimensional objects. [Nature] Vol.343 P.263-266 google cross ref
  • 38. Ratcliff R., McKoon G. 1995 Bias in the priming of object decisions. [Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition] Vol.21 P.754-767 google cross ref
  • 39. Roediger H. L., McDermott K. B. 1994 The problem of differing false-alarm rates for the process dissociation procedure: Comment on Verfaellie and Treadwell (1993). [Neuropsychology] Vol.8 P.284-288 google cross ref
  • 40. Schacter D. L., Cooper L. A. 1993 Implicit and explicit memory for novel visual objects: Structure and function. [Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition] Vol.19 P.995-1009 google cross ref
  • 41. Schacter D. L., Cooper L. A., Delaney S. M. 1990 Implicit memory for unfamiliar objects depends on access to structural descriptions. [Journal of Experimental Psychology: General] Vol.119 P.5-24 google cross ref
  • 42. Schacter D. L., Reiman E., Uecker A., Polster M. R., Yun L. S., Cooper L. A. 1995 Brain regions associated with retrieval of structurally coherent visual information. [Nature] Vol.376 P.587-590 google
  • 43. Scognamillo R., Rhodes G., Morrone C., Burr D. 2003 A feature-based model of symmetry detection. [Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B] Vol.270 P.1727-1733 google cross ref
  • 44. Sekuler A. B., Palmer S. E. 1992 Perception of partly occluded objects: A microgenetic analysis. [Journal of Experimental Psychology: General] Vol.121 P.95-111 google cross ref
  • 45. Simoncelli E. P., Olshausen B. A. 2001 Natural image statistics and neural representation. [Annual Reviews Neuroscience] Vol.24 P.1193-1216 google
  • 46. Smyth D., Willmore B., Thompson I. D., Baker G. E., Tolhurst D. J. 2003 The receptive-field organisation of simple cells in primary visual cortex (V1) of ferrets under natural scene stimulation. [Journal of Neuroscience] Vol.23 P.4746-4759 google
  • 47. Squire L. R. 1998 Memory Systems. [Academie des Sciences] Vol.321 P.153-156 google
  • 48. Stankiewicz B. J., Hummel J., Cooper E. E. 1998 The role of attention in priming for left-right reflections of object images: Evidence for a dual representation of object shape. [Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance] Vol.24 P.732-744 google cross ref
  • 49. Subramaniam S., Biederman I., Madigan S. A. 2000 Accurate identification but no priming and chance recognition memory for pictures in RSVP sequences. [Visual Cognition] Vol.7 P.511-535 google cross ref
  • 50. Tarr M. J. 1995 Rotating objects to recognize them: A case study of the role of viewpoint dependency in the recognition of three-dimensional objects. [Psychonomic Bulletin and Review] Vol.2 P.55-82 google cross ref
  • 51. Tarr M. J. 2003 Visual object recognition: Can a single mechanism suffice? In M. A. Peterson & G. Rhodes (Eds.), Perception of faces, objects, and scenes: Analytic and holistic processes P.177-211 google
  • 52. Touryan J., Felsen G., Dan Y. 2005 Spatial structure of complex cell receptive fields measured with natural images [Neuron] Vol.45 P.781-791 google cross ref
  • 53. Treisman A., Kanwisher N. 1998 Perceiving Visually-Presented Objects: Recognition, Awareness, and Modularity. [Current Opinion in Neurobiology] Vol.8 P.218-226 google cross ref
  • 54. Ullman S. 1996 High-level vision. google
  • 55. Ullman S. 1998 Three-dimensional object recognition based on the combination of views. [Cognition] Vol.67 P.21-44 google cross ref
  • 56. Van der Helm P. A., Leeuwenberg E. L. J. 1996 Goodness of visual regularities: A nontransformational approach. [Psychological Review] Vol.103 P.429-456 google cross ref
  • 57. Van Lier R. J., Leeuwenberg E. L. J., van der Helm P. A. 1997 In support of hierarchy in object representation. [Psychological Research] Vol.60 P.134-143 google cross ref
  • 58. Vetter T., Poggio T. 2002 Symmetric 3D objects are an easy case for 2D object recognition. In C. W. Tylor (Eds.), Human symmetry perception and its computational analysis P.349-359 google
  • 59. Vuong Q. C., Tarr M .J. 2004 Rotation direction affects object recognition. [Vision Research] Vol.44 P.1717-1730 google cross ref
  • 60. Vuong Q. C., Tarr M. J. 2006 Structural similarity and spatiotemporal noise effects on learning dynamic novel objects. [Perception] Vol.35 P.497-510 google cross ref
  • 61. Wagemans J. 1995 Detection of visual symmetries. [Spatial Vision] Vol.9 P.9-32 google cross ref
  • 62. Wagemans J. 1997 Characteristics and models of human symmetry detection. [Trends in cognitive Science] Vol.1 P.346-352 google cross ref
  • 63. Wenderoth P. 1997 The effects of bilateral-symmetry detection of multiple symmetry, near symmetry, and axis orientation. [Perception and Psychophysics] Vol.26 P.891-904 google
  • 64. Yamauchi T., Cooper L. A., John H. H., Szerlip N. J., Chen H. H., Barnhardt T. M. 2006 Priming for symmetry detection of three-dimensional figures: Central axes can prime symmetry detection separately from local components. [Visual Cognition] Vol.13 P.363-397 google cross ref
  • 65. Yuille A. L., Kersten D. 2006 Vision as Bayesian Inference: Analysis by Synthesis? [Trends in Cognitive Sciences] Vol.10 P.301-208 google cross ref
OAK XML 통계
이미지 / 테이블
  • [ Table 1. ]  A summary of the recognition memory tests in Experiment 2
    A summary of the recognition memory tests in Experiment 2
(우)06579 서울시 서초구 반포대로 201(반포동)
Tel. 02-537-6389 | Fax. 02-590-0571 | 문의 : oak2014@korea.kr
Copyright(c) National Library of Korea. All rights reserved.